Graduate Theses and Dissertations
Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and
Dissertations
2020
Does contextual information improve the detection of deceptive
Does contextual information improve the detection of deceptive
behavioral cues?
behavioral cues?
Kristen Alicia Slapinski
Iowa State University
Follow this and additional works at: https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd
Recommended Citation
Recommended Citation
Slapinski, Kristen Alicia, “Does contextual information improve the detection of deceptive behavioral
cues?” (2020). Graduate Theses and Dissertations. 18231.
https://lib.dr.iastate.edu/etd/18231
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Iowa State University Capstones, Theses and
Dissertations at Iowa State University Digital Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses and
Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Iowa State University Digital Repository. For more information,
please contact digirep@iastate.edu.
Does contextual information improve the detection of deceptive behavioral cues?
by
Kristen A. Slapinski
A thesis submitted to the graduate faculty
in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
MASTER OF SCIENCE
Major: Psychology
Program of Study Committee:
Stephanie Madon, Co-major Professor
Max Guyll, Co-major Professor
Zlatan Krizan
Christian Meissner
The student author, whose presentation of the scholarship herein was approved by the program
of study committee, is solely responsible for the content of this thesis. The Graduate College will
ensure this thesis is globally accessible and will not permit alterations after a degree is conferred.
Iowa State University
Ames, Iowa
2020
Copyright © Kristen A. Slapinski, 2020. All rights reserved.
ii
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page
LIST OF TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . iv
LIST OF FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . v
ABSTRACT . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . vi
CHAPTER 1: GENERAL INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
Background . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
Potential Cues to Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
Behavioral Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
Statement Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
Contextual Information and Deception Detection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
Increasing Reliance on Content Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
Providing an Opportunity to Detect Correspondence Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 8
Facilitating the Accurate Interpretation of Behavioral Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 9
Contextual Information and Schema Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
CHAPTER 2: OVERVIEW OF PROPOSED RESEARCH . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
Hypotheses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
Behavioral Cues . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
Contextual Information . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
Combined Effects of Behavioral Cues and Contextual Information . . . . . . . . . . . 16
Experiment Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 18
CHAPTER 3: PRELIMINARY STUDY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
Method . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 19
Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 20
Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 22
CHAPTER 4: METHODS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24
Power Analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24
Participants . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 25
Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26
Materials . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 26
Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 29
Procedure . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
iii
CHAPTER 5: RESULTS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 33
The Effects of Behavioral Cues and Contextual Information on Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . 33
Accuracy of the Dichotomous Judgment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 31
Accuracy of the Continuous Judgment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 36
The Effects of Ground Truth, Behavioral Cues, and Contextual Information
on Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38
Accuracy of the Dichotomous Judgment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 38
Accuracy of the Continuous Judgment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 41
Frequency of Truth or Deception Judgments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43
Dichotomous Judgment of Truth or Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45
Continuous Judgment of Truth or Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46
Effect of Including an Inconclusive Option . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
Use of Non-verbal Behavioral Cues vs. Verbal Information . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47
Relationship Between Individual Difference Measures, Accuracy,
and Judgments of Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48
CHAPTER 6: GENERAL DISCUSSION. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50
Hypotheses 1 and 2: Behavioral Cues Increase or Decrease Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 50
Hypothesis 3: Contextual Information Increases Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 51
Hypotheses 4 and 5: The Combined Effects of Behavioral Cues
and Contextual Information . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 55
The Effects of Ground Truth on Accuracy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 56
The Effects of Ground Truth, Behavioral Cues, and Contextual Information
on Judgments of Truth or Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58
Individual Difference Measures . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 64
Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 65
Implications . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 68
Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 70
REFERENCES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 73
TABLES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
FIGURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87
APPENDIX A: LOGIC PROBLEMS USED IN PRELIMINARY STUDY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
APPENDIX B: INSTRUCTIONS AND CONTEXTUAL INFORMATION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96
APPENDIX C: DEMOGRAPHIC QUESTIONNAIRE . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97
APPENDIX D: INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCE MEASURES . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98
APPENDIX E: MEASURES OF PARTICIPANT LIE-TELLING ABILITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102
APPENDIX F: INSTITUTIONAL REVIEW BOARD APPROVAL LETTER . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
iv
LIST OF TABLES
Page
Table 1 Accuracy of Judgments Across Ground Truth, Behavioral Cues,
and Contextual Information Conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
Table 2 Results of Logistic Regression Including Behavioral Cues and Contextual
Information Predicting Accuracy of Dichotomous Judgment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 80
Table 3 Results of an ANOVA Including Behavioral Cues and Contextual
Information Predicting Accuracy of Continuous Judgment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
Table 4 Results of Logistic Regression Including Ground Truth, Behavioral Cues,
and Contextual Information Predicting Accuracy of Dichotomous Judgment . . . . . . 82
Table 5 Results of an ANOVA Including Ground Truth, Behavioral Cues,
and Contextual Information Predicting Accuracy of Continuous Judgment . . . . . . . 83
Table 6 Results of Logistic Regression Including Ground Truth, Behavioral Cues,
and Contextual Information Predicting Dichotomous Judgments of
Truth or Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 84
Table 7 Results of an ANOVA Including Ground Truth, Behavioral Cues,
and Contextual Information Predicting Continuous Judgments of
Truth or Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
Table 8 Correlations and Descriptive Statistics for Individual Difference Measures,
Accuracy, and Judgments of Deception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 86
v
LIST OF FIGURES
Page
Figure 1 Accuracy of Dichotomous Judgments by Behavioral Cues,
Contextual Information Conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 87
Figure 2 Accuracy of Dichotomous Judgments by Ground Truth,
Behavioral Cues Conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 88
Figure 3 Accuracy of Dichotomous Judgments by Ground Truth,
Contextual Information Conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 89
Figure 4 Proportions of Dichotomous Judgments of Deception by Ground Truth,
Behavioral Cues, and Contextual Information Conditions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 90
Figure 5 Proportions of Dichotomous Judgments of Deception by
Ground Truth Conditions (p < .001) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 91
Figure 6 Proportions of Dichotomous Judgments of Deception by
Behavioral Cues Conditions (p = .035) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92
Figure 7 Proportions of Dichotomous Judgments of Deception by
Contextual Information Conditions (p < .001) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
vi
ABSTRACT
Influential interrogation manuals assert that investigators can detect deception from a
suspect’s behaviors with high rates of accuracy when they have access to contextual information
about the crime (e.g., case facts, witness statements, forensic evidence). The current study
provided the first empirical test of this claim. Undergraduate participants were presented with
statements made by either a liar or truth-teller who denied involvement in a real transgression
and judged whether they believed that the individual was lying or telling the truth. While making
this judgment, participants were either able or unable to observe the individual’s behavior and
received or did not receive relevant contextual information about the alleged transgression.
Results provided no evidence that the provision of behavioral cues or contextual information
affected accuracy of participants’ judgments independently or in combination. Exploratory
analyses revealed that behavioral cues biased judgments toward deception, while contextual
information biased judgments toward truth. These findings suggest that the evaluation of
behavioral cues may put innocent suspects at risk of being misclassified as deceptive, whereas
the provision of contextual information may bias judgments toward truth or exacerbate
individuals’ pre-existing biases.
1
CHAPTER 1. GENERAL INTRODUCTION
Proponents of influential interrogation methods claim that suspects leak behavioral cues
during questioning (e.g., fidgeting, hesitance, crossed-arms) that can be detected and interpreted
by investigators to distinguish truth from deception with high rates of accuracy (Inbau, Reid,
Buckley, & Jayne, 2013). Accordingly, the initial phase of an interrogation is often dedicated to
ascertaining whether suspects, on the basis of their behavior, are being truthful or deceptive.
Suspects deemed deceptive are often subjected to a coercive interrogation. Because coercive
interrogations can lead innocent suspects to falsely confess, and because false confessions are a
leading cause of wrongful conviction, it is critically important that innocent suspects not be
misclassified as deceptive on the basis of their behavior (Kassin, 2014).
However, the deception detection literature has repeatedly shown that people’s ability to
detect deception from behavioral cues is little better than chance, leading researchers to conclude
that the interpretation of a suspect’s behaviors is of little value for detecting deception (Bond &
DePaulo, 2006). Proponents of Reid-style interviewing and interrogation methods have argued
that studies showing near chance effects are not valid indicators of deception detection accuracy
due to limited ecological validity. For example, proponents of these methods partially attribute
the tendency of prior research to find near chance accuracy rates to the absence of relevant
contextual information about the event that is typically provided to investigators, and which
presumably promotes the accurate assessment of a suspect’s behavioral cues (see Inbau, Reid,
Buckley, & Jayne, 2013).
The objective of this research was to provide the first empirical test of the claim that the
presence of contextual information improves deception detection accuracy. The current chapter
addresses three issues that are directly relevant to this issue. First, it describes different sources
2
of cues that have the potential to aid in the detection of deception, including those that involve
the interpretation of behavioral cues and others that involve a content analysis of an individual’s
statements. Second, the chapter reviews research relevant to the claim that contextual
information increases deception detection accuracy. Finally, the chapter discusses a possible
psychological mechanism through which contextual information could increase the accuracy of
detecting deception from behavioral cues.
Background
In several personal and professional domains—politics, national security, marriage,
business, and police interrogation—the ability to accurately distinguish between truth and
deception is potentially advantageous. Unsurprisingly, deception and its detection have garnered
extensive attention in the psychological literature and generated considerable public interest.
However, the psychological literature indicates that laypeople and police investigators exhibit
deception detection abilities that are generally poor, and that there are no behaviors uniquely
associated with truth or deception (Bond & DePaulo, 2006).
In response to this finding, scholars have proposed that other cues to deception exist
within the content of an individual’s verbal statements that can be detected and interpreted with
higher accuracy than relying on the interpretation of behavioral cues alone. Indeed, there are
multiple sources of information that individuals can rely upon when attempting to detect
deception, including both the individual’s behaviors and the content of their spoken statements.
The following section reviews research relevant to two classes of cues to deception that have
been identified in the literature: behavioral cues and statement cues.
3
Potential Cues to Deception
The literature has identified two broad classes of cues that have the potential to reveal
deception: behavioral cues and statement cues. Behavioral cues include non-verbal and
paraverbal behaviors. Statement cues include content cues and correspondence cues. The
following sections describe behavioral cues and statement cues in more detail.
Behavioral Cues
In the typical deception detection paradigm, some participants (judges) evaluate the
veracity of statements made by other participants (senders) on the basis of non-verbal (e.g., body
language) and paraverbal (e.g., voice tone, pitch, hesitancy) behavioral cues. The interpretation
of behavioral cues to deception rests on the assumption that deception is accompanied by
feelings of fear, guilt, or excitement that manifest themselves in a sender’s non-verbal and
paraverbal behaviors (Trovillo, 1939; Ekman, 1985). For instance, according to the theory of
emotional leakage (Ekman & Friesen, 1969), behavioral cues to deception can be detected via
microexpressions of excitement, fear, or guilt that flash across a sender’s face for milliseconds.
Building upon this assumption, other theories have attempted to identify the conditions
under which these behavioral cues are more likely to emerge. For example, some theories
hypothesize that behavioral cues to deception are more likely when a liar experiences heightened
emotions or cognitive load (Zuckerman, DePaulo, & Rosenthal, 1981), when deception is
attempted in high stakes situations (DePaulo, Ansfield, & Bell, 1996), and in response to certain
investigator behaviors (Buller & Burgoon, 1996). The interpretation of behavioral cues is
recommended in police interrogation manuals (see Inbau, Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2013) and
laypeople generally believe that behavioral cues to deception exist, such as the belief that liars
avoid eye contact (Taylor & Hick, 2007; Vrij, Akehurst, & Knight, 2006).
4
However, the scientific study of deception detection indicates that deception detection
accuracy is only slightly above chance. Most notably, a meta-analysis of 206 deception detection
studies found that judges accurately classified lies and truths at a rate of 54%, only 4% better
than what would be expected by chance alone (Bond & DePaulo, 2006). There are two related
explanations that may account for this finding. First, liars and truth-tellers can experience similar
feelings of fear and stress, suggesting that behavioral cues indicative of internal emotions are
unreliable indicators of deception (DePaulo, Lindsay, Malone, Muhlenbruck, Charlton, &
Cooper, 2003; Ofshe & Leo, 1997). Second, the actual behaviors of liars versus truth-tellers tend
to be more similar than different, and the few behavioral cues that do distinguish between them
tend to be faint (see DePaulo et al., 2003; Sporer & Schwandt, 2006, 2007).
Consistent with these findings, there is some indication in the empirical literature that the
presence of behavioral cues can be detrimental to deception detection accuracy. In particular,
there is evidence that judges become less accurate at detecting deception when non-verbal
behavioral cues are present than when they are absent (Anderson, DePaulo, Ansfield, Tickle, &
Green, 1999; Bond & DePaulo, 2006; Mann, Vrij, & Bull, 2004). For example, when judges
assessed audio or videotaped statements of senders either truthfully or deceptively confessing to
crime, accuracy rates were lower for those assessing videotaped statements that included both
paraverbal and non-verbal behaviors than those assessing audiotaped statements that included
only paraverbal behaviors (Kassin, Meissner, & Norwick, 2005). Thus, the addition of non-
verbal behaviors decreased accuracy. Similarly, while paraverbal behaviors do not appear to be
particularly detrimental, they do not increase deception detection accuracy. For example, judges
are no more accurate at identifying deception from audio or audiovisual presentations of a
sender’s statements than they are at identifying deception from transcripts of a sender’s
5
statements (Bond & DePaulo, 2006). The findings that non-verbal behaviors are detrimental to
accuracy and paraverbal behaviors are largely unhelpful raises the possibility that the
interpretation of behavioral cues may generally be detrimental to deception detection accuracy.
Statement Cues
In response to mounting evidence that judges are not able to accurately distinguish
between truth and deception through the interpretation of behavioral cues alone, some
researchers have suggested that higher accuracy rates might be obtained if judges based their
evaluations on statement cues – i.e., cues to deception contained within the verbal content of
statements (Blair, Levine, & Shaw, 2010; Köhnken, 1987; Reinhard, Sporer, Scharmach, &
Marksteiner, 2011). Importantly, the interpretation of statement cues to deception does not
necessitate the observation of behavior. This is because statement cues can be available even
when behavioral cues are absent, such as when judges are presented with a transcript of a
sender’s statements. As briefly noted above, the literature has distinguished between two types of
statement cues: content cues and correspondence cues.
Content Cues
Content cues refer to the verbal content of a sender’s statement, such as the amount of
detail, consistency, completeness, and vividness the statement conveys (Reinhard, Sporer,
Scharmach, & Marksteiner, 2011). Research shows that liars' and truth-tellers' statements differ
in terms of content cues. Liars, for instance, make statements that are less detailed, consistent,
and plausible than those made by truth-tellers (DePaulo et al., 2003).
There is increasing evidence that relying on the analysis of content cues improves
deception detection accuracy (Reinhard, Sporer, Scharmach, & Marksteiner, 2011; Reinhard,
Sporer, & Scharmach, 2013), and this has led researchers to develop new techniques to aid in
6
deception detection. For example, content-based criteria analysis, which was developed
specifically for cases of suspected child sexual abuse, assesses truthfulness by analyzing the
content cues within a child’s statement, such as the degree to which the statements are
characterized by a logical structure and unusual or superfluous details (Steller & Köhnken,
1989). Empirical research has shown that truthful versus deceptive accounts differ on these
criteria, thereby supporting the efficacy of using this method to distinguish between truth and
deception (Lamb et al., 1997; Vrij, 2005; Vrij, Akehurst, Soukara, & Bull, 2002). Similarly, the
verifiability approach trains investigators to ask suspects to provide verifiable details about their
alibi. Because deceptive suspects tend to provide fewer verifiable details than truth-tellers,
assessing this content cue increases deception detection accuracy (Nahari, Vrij, & Fisher, 2012;
Vrij & Nahari, 2019). Overall, the effectiveness of these methods suggest that assessing content
cues, such as the number of details reported, can increase deception detection accuracy rates.
Correspondence Cues
By contrast, correspondence cues are cues to deception that are derived from an analysis
of consistency between the verbal content of a sender’s statements and contextual information
about the crime (e.g., evidence, witness statements). Presumably, truthful senders will tend to
make statements that correspond to the contextual information, whereas deceptive senders will
tend to make statements that do not correspond to the contextual information. For example, if the
police identified a suspect’s fingerprints at a crime scene, but the suspect denies ever having
been there, then the investigator can infer that the suspect is probably lying. Investigators often
rely on correspondence cues to arrive at judgments of deception (Granhag & Strömwall, 1999;
Granhag & Strömwall, 2002; Granhag, Strömwall, & Jonsson, 2003), and empirical research has
7
demonstrated that judges achieve superior accuracy rates when they are able to interpret
correspondence cues (Blair, Levine, & Shaw, 2010; Blair, Reimer, & Levine, 2018).
Contextual Information and Deception Detection
The provision of crime-related contextual information – for instance, the provision of
case facts, witness statements, and/or forensic evidence – has the potential to increase deception
detection accuracy through three mechanisms: (1) by increasing judges’ reliance on content cues;
(2) by providing judges the opportunity to detect correspondence cues; (3) by facilitating the
accurate interpretation of behavioral cues to deception. These mechanisms are described in detail
below.
Increasing Reliance on Content Cues
There is some evidence that the provision of crime-relevant contextual information can
improve the interpretation of content cues to deception. For example, judges who were provided
with contextual information reported relying more heavily on content cues to deception, and
thereby demonstrated greater deception detection accuracy than judges who were not provided
with contextual information (Reinhard, Sporer, Scharmach, & Marksteiner, 2011; Reinhard,
Sporer, & Scharmach, 2013). Importantly, this effect occurred even though the contextual
information contained no information that could be directly compared to the content of the
suspect’s statement. This suggests that the provision of relevant contextual information may
increase judges’ perceived familiarity with a situation. In turn, because judges feel more familiar
with the situation, they may be more likely to attend to content cues to deception over behavioral
cues. The tendency to rely more on diagnostic content cues than behavioral cues may enable
judges to better distinguish between liars and truth-tellers.
8
Providing an Opportunity to Detect Correspondence Cues
By definition, the interpretation of correspondence cues to deception necessitates having
knowledge of crime-relevant contextual information. Accordingly, contextual information
increases deception detection accuracy by creating the potential for correspondence cues to
signal when the verbal content of a sender’s statement fails to correspond to known contextual
information (Blair, Levine, & Shaw, 2010; Blair, Reimer, & Levine, 2018; Reinhard, Sporer,
Scharmach, & Marksteiner, 2011). For example, in an alleged cheating incident, judges who
were provided with contextual information indicating that a set of logic problems were so
difficult that no one could get more than two correct without cheating achieved a higher
deception detection accuracy rate than those who were not provided with this contextual
information (Blair, Levine, & Shaw, 2010). Overall, this suggests that the interpretation of
correspondence cues increases the accuracy of deception detection judgments independently of
behavioral cues and content cues.
Recognizing the potential utility of correspondence cues, researchers have developed
several deception detection techniques that emphasize the importance of attending to
correspondence cues. The strategic use of evidence technique, for example, trains judges to use
contextual information to elicit correspondence cues to deception, leading to an increase in
deception detection accuracy. Specifically, judges are trained to acquire relevant evidence and
contextual information pertinent to the suspect and the crime prior to an interview, to ask
increasingly specific questions about the withheld evidence during the interview, and only at the
end of the interview to reveal the evidence they have. The objective is to keep senders unaware
of known evidence in order to elicit more statement-evidence inconsistencies that are likely to be
indicative of deception (see Granhag, Strömwall, Willen, & Hartwig, 2012; Hartwig, Granhag,
9
Strömwall, & Kronkvist, 2006; Hartwig, Granhag, Strömwall, & Vrij, 2005). Judges who use
this technique exhibit superior accuracy compared to those who do not use the technique
(Hartwig, Granhag, Strömwall, & Kronkvist, 2006).
The effectiveness of using contextual information to detect correspondence cues to
deception is also supported by other deception detection methods. For example, investigators
using cognitive load approaches are trained to increase the suspect’s attentional demands in order
to elicit contradictions between their statements and known contextual information, thereby
increasing deception detection accuracy (Vrij, Fisher, Mann, & Leal, 2008). Overall, this
suggests that training methods that elicit correspondence cues between the content of a sender’s
statement and contextual information can improve deception detection accuracy.
Facilitating the Accurate Interpretation of Behavioral Cues
A third mechanism by which contextual information could increase accuracy is through
the interpretation of behavioral cues. According to proponents of the Reid Technique of
Interviewing and Interrogation—the most commonly used police interrogation manual in the
world—providing contextual information to judges increases deception detection accuracy by
facilitating the degree to which they accurately interpret suspects’ non-verbal and paraverbal
behavioral cues to deception (Inbau, Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2013). For example, to address
criticisms of their behavior-based deception detection methods, authors of the training manual
(Inbau, Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2013) argue that the ability for judges to accurately interpret
behavioral cues to deception increases when they are provided with contextual information:
“When an investigator understands the context in which an interview is taking place (for
example, the case facts and background information) accuracy in the assessment of a subject’s
behavior symptoms greatly increases.” (p. 103).
10
Moreover, the authors claim that many deception detection studies have failed to yield
high accuracy rates, in part, because: “There was little consideration given to evaluating
behaviors in context. For example, identifying whether specific non-verbal behaviors are
appropriate given the verbal content of the suspect’s response, identifying the consistency of the
suspect’s statement across time and with known evidence, and so on.” (p. 104). Thus, while the
authors do note that contextual information may increase accuracy through mechanisms such as
content or correspondence cues, they additionally argue that contextual information should orient
the investigator’s attention toward specific non-verbal behaviors indicative of deception. In other
words, the authors of the training manual do not merely argue that contextual information
improves deception detection by orienting the investigator’s attention toward content cues, or by
initiating a comparison between contextual information and the content of a suspect’s statement.
Instead, they argue that contextual information provides the foundation for the accurate
interpretation of behavioral cues to deception.
Presuming this to be true, the training manual specifically instructs police to use case-
relevant contextual information when interpreting a suspect’s behavioral cues for deception. In
fact, the manual includes a non-verbal lie detection method of questioning, the Behavior
Analysis Interview—a non-accusatory interview conducted before an interrogation in which
investigators are encouraged to interpret a suspect’s behaviors in order to determine if the
suspect is being truthful or deceptive. This evaluation is then used as the basis for deciding
whether or not to proceed with an accusatory interrogation.
The behavioral analysis interview involves first collecting background information from
the suspect in order to establish a sense of the suspects’ normative or baseline behaviors. Then,
the interview proceeds by asking the suspect a series of fifteen behavior-provoking questions.
11
For example, in an alleged theft case, behavior-provoking questions might include “Do you
know who stole the money?” and “Have you ever thought about stealing money?”. According to
the manual, these behavior-provoking questions mobilize guilty individuals, thereby increasing
physiological arousal and excitement. For example, compared to innocent suspects, guilty
suspects undergoing a behavior analysis interview are expected to be less helpful and sincere and
to exhibit more behaviors indicative of nervousness, such as by appearing more guarded, having
a closed, static, and non-frontal body posture, and displaying less illustrative behaviors with their
hands (Inbau, Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2013). Because these questions are intended to elicit
different behavioral responses from innocent and guilty suspects, proponents of these methods
argue that by conducting a behavior analysis interview, investigators are able to reliably detect
and interpret behavioral cues indicative of truth or deception in a manner akin to detecting
physiological arousal with a polygraph (Horvath & Jayne, 1990).
Despite widespread use of this behavior-based lie detection tool, little research has been
conducted that has supported its effectiveness. Initial research conducted by affiliates of the
training manual reported that investigators using the behavior analysis interview achieved high
deception detection accuracy rates (Blair & McCamey, 2002; Horvath, Jayne, & Buckley, 1994).
However, several methodological limitations, including that the ground truth of senders’
statements could not be established, have led scholars to question the validity of the findings
(Masip, Herrero, Garrido, & Barba, 2011; Vrij, Mann, & Fisher, 2006). In another study in
which ground truth was established by experimentally manipulating the sender’s guilt status,
innocent suspects undergoing a behavior analysis interview exhibited more nervous behaviors
and were less helpful to investigators than guilty suspects, a finding that is in direct opposition of
the claims made by the training manual (Vrij, Mann, & Fisher, 2006). Thus, the results from the
12
limited empirical research examining the behavior analysis interview have not conclusively
established its effectiveness.
In addition to the lack of empirical support, there is no research that supports or refutes
the validity of the training manual’s claim that contextual information increases the accuracy of
behavioral cue interpretation. Although contextual information can increase the deception
detection accuracy rate, it is unclear whether the mechanisms underlying this effect are solely
due to the interpretation of statement cues, or also due to the interpretation of behavioral cues. In
particular, because no research has simultaneously manipulated the presence or absence of
contextual information and the presence or absence of behavioral cues, it is impossible to know
whether the increase in deception detection accuracy that is found when contextual information
is provided could be partially attributed to an increase in the accurate interpretation of behavioral
cues (Blair, Levine, & Shaw, 2010; Blair, Reimer, & Levine, 2018).
Consistent with the training manual’s claims, contextual information might increase
deception detection accuracy by facilitating the degree to which people correctly interpret
another’s behavioral cues to deception. If this is the case, then the presence of behavioral cues
should increase deception detection accuracy above and beyond any effects attributable to
contextual information. Alternatively, contextual information might only increase deception
detection accuracy by increasing people’s reliance on content and correspondence cues to
deception, such as by leading them to compare a suspect’s verbal statements to available
contextual information. If this is the case, then accuracy in deception detection judgments may
actually be greatest when behavioral cues (that might otherwise mislead or distract) are absent.
Distinguishing between these alternative explanations requires research that orthogonally
13
manipulates both contextual information and behavioral cues to deception, which has not
previously been done.
Contextual Information and Schema Theory
Although authors of the training manual did not offer a theoretical explanation as to why
contextual information might facilitate the accurate interpretation of a suspect’s behavioral cues,
schema theory provides a mechanism that could provide support for the training manual’s claim.
Specifically, contextual information could activate knowledge structures that guide how
individuals encode and interpret incoming information, and in turn affect memory and
comprehension (see Bransford & Johnson, 1992; Taylor & Crocker, 1981; Trope, 1986). In this
way, schematic information processing may guide attention away from irrelevant perceptual
details and toward more relevant information (von Hippel, Jonides, Hilton, & Narayan, 1993).
With respect to police interrogation, for example, contextual information about a crime could
activate a schematic representation of the criminal event. In turn, this schematic representation
may organize and guide an investigator’s attention away from irrelevant behavioral cues, and
conversely, toward diagnostic behavioral cues to deception, thereby increasing deception
detection accuracy.
Schematic information processing also simplifies cognitive processing and frees up
attentional resources to attend to other information (Macrae, Milne, & Bodenhausen, 1994).
Accordingly, a schema that is activated by crime-relevant contextual information may reduce the
cognitive resources an investigator needs to understand the background of a case via the
suspect’s spoken statement, thereby allowing the investigator to allocate more of their cognitive
resources to the interpretation of behavioral cues. The availability of these cognitive resources
may enable the investigator to allocate more of their information processing resources to
14
detecting and interpreting a suspect’s subtler behavioral cues to deceit, thereby improving the
investigator’s ability to accurately detect deception.
15
CHAPTER 2. OVERVIEW OF PROPOSED RESEARCH
Hypotheses
This experiment tested several hypotheses relevant to the effects of behavioral cues and
contextual information on deception detection accuracy.
Behavioral Cues
I tested competing, directional predictions about the effect of behavioral cues on
deception detection.
Hypothesis 1: Behavioral Cues Increase Accuracy
One the one hand, theories of emotional leakage (e.g., Ekman & Friesen, 1969) propose
that deceptive individuals show behaviors that can be detected by observers. Accordingly, this
perspective predicts judges should exhibit greater deception detection accuracy when behavioral
cues are present than when they are absent.
Hypothesis 2: Behavioral Cues Decrease Accuracy
On the other hand, empirical research has demonstrated that deception detection
accuracy is greater when judges are provided with transcribed or audiotaped statements than
when they are provided with videotaped statements (e.g., Kassin, Meissner, & Norwick, 2005),
perhaps because visual behaviors distract judges from statement cues to deception. This view
corresponds to the prediction that judges should exhibit greater deception detection accuracy
when behavioral cues are absent than when they are present.
Contextual Information
I made a single, directional hypothesis with respect to the effect of contextual
information on deception detection accuracy.
16
Hypothesis 3: Contextual Information Increases Accuracy
Specifically, contextual information should increase reliance on statement cues and
thereby increase deception detection accuracy (e.g., Blair, Levine, & Shaw, 2010; Blair,
Reimer, & Levine, 2018). Thus, I predicted that when contextual information is present, judges
should exhibit greater deception detection accuracy than when contextual information is absent.
Combined Effects of Behavioral Cues and Contextual Information
This experiment tested competing, directional predictions about the combined effects of
behavioral cues and contextual information on deception detection accuracy.
Hypothesis 4: Contextual Information Improves Interpretation of Behavioral Cues
Proponents of the interviewing methods presented in the Reid training manual assert
that contextual information should improve the interpretation of behavioral cues to deception,
further increasing accuracy (see Inbau, Reid, Buckley, & Jayne, 2013). This perspective
corresponds to an interaction, such that the improvement in accuracy created by the presence of
behavioral cues is greater when contextual information is present than when contextual
information is absent. However, it is not feasible to test for the presence of an interaction effect
in this sample with sufficient power. For example, if I were to predict that the absence of
contextual information would attenuate the effect of behavioral cues on accuracy by 50%, I
would need to collect 14 times the number of participants that I would need to test for a main
effect of behavioral cues (N = 448), resulting in a total sample size of 6,272 (Giner-Sorolla,
2018).
Therefore, given a non-significant interaction term, I will further test the hypothesis that
contextual information improves the interpretation of behavioral cues through a pairwise
comparison that will assess whether accuracy rates follow the predicted pattern. Specifically,
17
this perspective would predict that when contextual information is present, accuracy will be
greater when behavioral cues are present than when they are absent. By contrast, this
perspective does not correspond to any clear prediction about the effect of behavioral cues on
accuracy when contextual information is absent. Therefore, if accuracy follows the predicted
pattern when contextual information is present, I will informally compare the sizes of the
differences in accuracy when behavioral cues are absent versus present to assess whether the
difference is greater when contextual information is present.
Hypothesis 5: Behavioral Cues Distract from Interpretation of Contextual Information
As detailed above, empirical evidence suggests that the provision of behavioral cues
worsens deception detection (e.g., Kassin, Meissner, & Norwick, 2005), whereas the provision
of contextual information improves deception detection (e.g., Blair, Levine, & Shaw, 2010).
These results pertaining to their separate effects suggest two specific predictions about how they
could operate in combination to affect deception detection accuracy. Specifically, when
contextual information is provided, one would expect accuracy to be greater. However, the
additional provision of behavioral cues may distract judges from statement cues and decrease
accuracy. This perspective corresponds to an interaction, such that the improvement in accuracy
created by the presence of contextual information is greater when behavioral cues are absent
than when they are present. However, for the reasons described above, it is not feasible to test
for the presence of an interaction in this sample with sufficient power. For this reason, I will use
the same analytic approach described above. Namely, given a non-significant interaction term, I
will further test the hypothesis that behavioral cues distract or mislead judges from accurately
evaluating contextual information through two pairwise comparisons, followed by informal
comparisons of the sizes of differences in accuracy.
18
In particular, this perspective would predict that when contextual information is present,
accuracy will be greater when behavioral cues are absent than when they are present. It should
be noted that this prediction is opposite to the prediction implied by proponents of the Reid
training manual, which I articulated above. Furthermore, when behavioral cues are absent, one
would expect no distraction from the beneficial effect expected by providing contextual
information. Therefore, this perspective would also predict that when behavioral cues are
absent, accuracy will be greater when contextual information is present than when it is absent. If
accuracy follows the predicted pattern, I will informally compare the sizes of the differences in
accuracy when contextual information is absent versus present to assess whether the difference
is greater when behavioral cues are absent.
Experiment Overview
This experiment provided the first test of whether the provision of contextual information
increases the accuracy with which judges can interpret behavioral cues to deception. To do so,
the experiment manipulated the presence of contextual information and the presence of
behavioral cues in order to isolate their unique effects. Participants were presented with
statements made by a liar or truth-teller proclaiming innocence in an alleged cheating incident.
These statements were either presented in the form of an audiotaped transcript narrated by a text-
to-speech program in which only statement cues were present, or in the form of a videotape that
communicated both the statement cues as well as any behavioral cues the liar or truth-teller
might have leaked. Further, contextual information about the cheating incident was either
withheld from or provided to participants.